Comparative psychology

In a broader meaning, comparative psychology includes comparisons between different biological and socio-cultural groups, such as species, sexes, developmental stages, ages, and ethnicities.

Donald Dewsbury reviewed the works of several psychologists and their definitions and concluded that the object of comparative psychology is to establish principles of generality focusing on both proximate and ultimate causation.

Darwin's theory led to several hypotheses, one being that the factors that set humans apart, such as higher mental, moral and spiritual faculties, could be accounted for by evolutionary principles.

In response to the vehement opposition to Darwinism was the "anecdotal movement" led by George Romanes who set out to demonstrate that animals possessed a "rudimentary human mind".

Douglas Alexander Spalding was called the "first experimental biologist",[4] and worked mostly with birds; studying instinct, imprinting, and visual and auditory development.

[4] Although the field initially attempted to include a variety of species, by the early 1950s it had focused primarily on the white lab rat and the pigeon, and the topic of study was restricted to learning, usually in mazes.

However, the broader use of the term "comparative psychology" is enshrined in the names of learned societies and academic journals, not to mention in the minds of psychologists of other specialisms, so the label of the field is never likely to disappear completely.

These attempts floundered; in retrospect it can be seen that they were not sufficiently sophisticated, either in their analysis of the demands of different tasks, or in their choice of species to compare.

Thorndike began his studies with cats, but American comparative psychologists quickly shifted to the more economical rat, which remained the almost invariable subject for the first half of the 20th century and continues to be used.

Due to their domestic nature and personalities, dogs have lived closely with humans, and parallels in communication and cognitive behaviours have therefore been recognised and further researched.

For example, there is extensive research with animals on attention, categorization, concept formation, memory, spatial cognition, and time estimation.

Much research in these and other areas is related directly or indirectly to behaviors important to survival in natural settings, such as navigation, tool use, and numerical competence.

[18][19][20] Veterinary surgeons recognize that the psychological state of a captive or domesticated animal must be taken into account if its behavior and health are to be understood and optimized.

[23] For example, rats who are exposed to loud music for a long period will ultimately develop unwanted behaviors that have been compared with human psychosis, like biting their owners.

[citation needed] The way dogs behave when understimulated is widely believed to depend on the breed as well as on the individual animal's character.

Ingold quoted Sigmund Freud: "Children show no trace of arrogance which urges adult civilized men to draw a hard-and-fast line between their own nature and that of all other animals.

The New York Times ran an article that showed the psychological benefits of animals,[28] more specifically of children with their pets.

In the article, Dr. Sue Doescher, a psychologist involved in the study, stated, "It made the children more cooperative and sharing."

Furthermore, in an edition of Social Science and Medicine it was stated, "A random survey of 339 residents from Perth, Western Australia were selected from three suburbs and interviewed by telephone.

Pet ownership was found to be positively associated with some forms of social contact and interaction, and with perceptions of neighborhood friendliness.

After adjustment for demographic variables, pet owners scored higher on social capital and civic engagement scales.

photo of a kitty
this cat is an expert in comparative psychology