Cognitive geography

These improved models assist in a variety of issues, for example, developing maps that communicate better, providing navigation instructions that are easier to follow, utilizing space more practically, accounting for the cultural differences on spatial thinking for more effective cross-cultural information exchange, and an overall increased understanding of our environment.

Notable researchers in this branch of geography include David Mark, Daniel Montello, Max J. Egenhofer, Andrew U. Frank, Christian Freksa, Edward Tolman, and Barbara Tversky, among others.

More behaviorally oriented geographers are materialists and look at the role of basic learning processes and how they influence the landscape patterns or even group identity.

[6] In the early 70s, the focus was on how to improve maps by providing useful information, delivering an understandable message, and making it more aesthetically pleasing.

This research area aims to minimize the disparity between the environment and its geometric representation and remove inherent spatial cognitive biases.

Examples of spatial cognitive biases include overestimating the distance between two locations when there are many intersections and nodes in the path.

David Mark through his research illustrates how spatial features like inland water bodies (lakes, ponds, lagoons) are categorized differently in English and French-speaking population and, therefore, could cause issues in cross-cultural geographical information exchange.

For example, symbols, their color, and their relative size have an important role to play in the interaction between the map and the mapmaker.

Diagram showing elements of spatial contextual (or geographic) awareness.